How much have you learned? - PSAT reading passage strategies - PSAT Reading

PSAT/NMSQT Prep 2022 - Eggert M.D., Strelka A. 2022

How much have you learned?
PSAT reading passage strategies
PSAT Reading

Directions: Take five minutes to actively read the following passage by 1) noting the keywords, 2) jotting down margin notes next to each paragraph, and 3) summarizing the big picture. When you’re done, compare your work to the Answers and Explanations at the end of the chapter.

This passage, about infant language acquisition, is adapted from a research paper written in 2017 that explored early childhood development.

1Infants are born as scientists, 2constantly interacting with and 3questioning the world around them. 4However, as any good scientist 5knows, simply making observations 6is not sufficient; a large part of 7learning is dependent on being able 8to communicate ideas, observations, 9and feelings with others. Though most 10infants do not produce discernible 11words until around age one or one-anda- 12half, they begin gaining proficiency 13in their native languages long before 14that. In fact, many linguists agree that a 15newborn baby’s brain is already preprogrammed 16for language acquisition, 17meaning that it’s as natural for a baby 18to talk as it is for a dog to dig.

19According to psycholinguist Anne 20Cutler, an infant’s language acquisition 21actually begins well before birth. At 22only one day old, newborns 23have demonstrated the ability to 24recognize the voices and rhythms heard 25during their last trimester in the 26muffled confines of the womb. 27In general, infants are more likely to 28attend to a specific voice stream if they 29perceive it as more familiar than other 30streams. Newborns tend to be especially 31partial to their mother’s voice and her 32native language, as opposed to another 33woman or another language. For 34example, when an infant is presented 35with a voice stream spoken by his 36mother and a background stream 37delivered by an unfamiliar voice, he 38will effortlessly attend to his mother 39while ignoring the background stream. 40Therefore, by using these simple yet 41important cues, and others like them, 42infants can easily learn the essential 43characteristics and rules of their native 44language.

45However, it is important to note 46that an infant’s ability to learn from 47the nuances of her mother’s speech is 48predicated upon her ability to separate 49that speech from the sounds of the 50dishwasher, the family dog, the bus 51stopping on the street outside, and, 52quite possibly, other streams of speech, 53like a newscaster on the television 54down the hall or siblings playing in 55an adjacent room. Infants are better 56able to accomplish this task when the 57voice of interest is louder than any 58of the competing background noises. 59Conversely, when two voices are of 60equal amplitude, infants typically 61demonstrate little preference for one 62stream over the other. Researchers have 63hypothesized that because an infant’s 64ability to selectively pay attention to 65one voice or sound, even in a mix of 66others, has not fully developed yet, the 67infant is actually interpreting competing 68voice streams that are equally loud 69as one single stream with unfamiliar 70patterns and sounds.

71During the first few months after 72birth, infants will subconsciously 73study the language being used around 74them, taking note of the rhythmic 75patterns, the sequences of sounds, 76and the intonation of the language. 77Newborns will also start to actively 78process how things like differences 79in pitch or accented syllables further 80affect meaning. Interestingly, up 81until six months of age, they can still 82recognize and discriminate between 83the phonemes (single units of sound in 84a language like “ba” or “pa”) of other 85languages. Though infants do display a 86preference for the language they heard 87in utero, most infants are not biased 88towards the specific phonemes of that 89language.

90This ability to recognize and 91discriminate between all phonemes 92comes to an end by the middle of their 93first year, at which point infants start 94displaying a preference for phonemes 95in their native language, culminating 96at age one, when they stop responding 97to foreign phonemes altogether. This is 98part of what is known as the “critical 99period,” which begins at birth and 100lasts until puberty. During this 101period, as the brain continues to grow 102and change, language acquisition is 103instinctual, explaining why young 104children seem to pick up languages so 105easily.