Realistic space flight - Sometimes, it really is rocket science

Putting the science in fiction - Dan Koboldt, Chuck Wendig 2018

Realistic space flight
Sometimes, it really is rocket science

By Sylvia Spruck Wrigley

Science fictional space flight involves three steps forward and two steps back. Swooping through the sky in a one- or two-man cockpit, surrounded by windows for a clear view of the starry-filled sky, a meaty joystick for the controls. The pilots are obviously the epitome of cool (this is of course standard for any pilot!), and piloting a spaceship looks suspiciously similar to a video game. The pilot isn’t focused on computers and instruments but reliant on fast hand-eye coordination and quick thinking. Space flight is all about the maneuverability, as our trusty pilot stares out the windows to see what’s coming.

If there’s a fleet, they are all oriented the same way up and neatly lined up. They apparently do this by instinctively knowing the level plane on which they are flying, as if all of the ships were skimming a great invisible lake.

It’s true that, if you get your private pilot’s license, you will be taught to fly visually: that is, to keep your bearings by staring out the window. Once you progress from there, however, it’s all about the glass cockpit and the instruments. Tell fighter-jet pilots that they need to maneuver by looking out the window and they’ll laugh you out of the room. That just isn’t fast enough.

In our universe, the reason an aircraft banks as it turns is to change the lift force: As the aircraft inclines toward the side of the turn, the lift force gains a horizontal component that forces the aircraft to turn. However, in space, you are flying in a vacuum and there is no lift force.

Illusions and disorientation in space

The most common cause of death for the solo pilot at high altitudes isn’t crashing in a high-speed obstacle course. The real risk is falling prey to illusions. The problem with flying is that, if you aren’t able to see the ground and a horizon, your brain gets confused and lies to you.

This is why the basic private pilot’s license is bound by visual flight rules, which only allows for flight within sight of the land and with clear visibility. The next stage of training is the instrument rating, which, as the name implies, trains the pilot to rely on the instruments in the cockpit rather than believing his eyes.

If you aren’t instrument-rated and you fly into a cloud, losing sight of the ground, you are in trouble. Statistics from the National Transportation Safety Board show that you are unlikely to survive. Your life expectancy drops from decades to 178 seconds.

So here’s the thing: Flying in space is a lot like flying in cloud. You have no point of reference for up and down and whether you are flying straight on or in a turn. And once you lose these references, you succumb to disorientation. Technically, once you fly away from your colonized planet, if you are still staring outside instead of relying on your flight computer, you are likely to get tangled into an illusion and turn your spacecraft upside down in less than three minutes.

You cannot stop the illusion; the best you can do is train yourself to ignore it. This means disregarding everything your brain and body are telling you and believing the instruments. You need to be looking in, not out.

It’s not just humans who have this issue, by the way. Radar studies have shown that, although European starlings can maintain straight and level flight when flying in overcast conditions, birds flying within the clouds fly erratically, suggesting that they are equally disoriented in the absence of visual cues.

Once you lose that visual frame of reference, your brain lies. Luckily, it lies consistently enough that we’ve learned to catch it out.

The graveyard spiral is probably the most well-known example of dangerous spatial disorientation. Common in poor weather conditions and night flights, the pilot loses the ability to judge the orientation of the plane.

What happens is that, without visual reference, you enter a banking turn. You initially can “feel” the turn, let’s say to the left. However, if the turn continues for twenty seconds or more, you’ll end up feeling that the aircraft is no longer turning. It feels absolutely as if you were flying straight with the wings level.

If you try to continue the turn, you’ll go into a much steeper left turn. More likely, you’ll want to be sure you have straightened out, but the straight-and-level aircraft will feel to you as if the aircraft is turning and banking to the right, even though you are now wings level. The compelling belief that you are now turning right will lead you to go back into the left turn. And the plane very slowly and gently flies in circles, a downward spiral that tightens as the aircraft descends. Your speed will increase until eventually you will escape the clouds and/or impact the ground.

Here’s what’s happening in the body to cause this disorientation according to Medical Facts for Pilots from the Federal Aviation Administration (www.faa.gov/pilots/safety/pilotsafetybrochures/media/SpatialD.pdf):

In this kind of turn, the fluid inside the canal starts moving initially, then friction causes it to catch up with the walls of the rotating canal. When this happens, the hairs inside the canal will return to their straight up position, sending an erroneous signal to the brain that the turn has stopped—when, in fact, the turn continues.

If you then start rolling out of the turn to go back to level flight, the fluid inside the canal will continue to move (because of its inertia), and the hairs will now move in the opposite direction, sending an erroneous signal to the brain indicating that you are turning in the opposite direction, when in fact, you are actually slowing down from the original turn.

The problem with a graveyard spiral in space is that its very existence means you’re generating mild gravity as you tighten the spin. But honestly, if we start thinking hard about gravity within our dog-fighting spacecraft, and if we pay too much attention to the science, they’ll all implode into a black hole of improbability anyway.

More pertinent for our science fiction stories, then, are the visual illusions that aren’t reliant on problematic laws of physics.

Autokinetic effect gives the pilot the impression that a stationary object is moving. Specifically, a stationary small point of light in a dark or featureless environment, like a star-filled sky in space. This illusion is caused by very small movements of the eyes, which the brain misinterprets as movement of the stationary object. It means that we would find it incredibly difficult to tell the difference between a star and an approaching spaceship.

In Generation Kill (Putnam Adult, 2004), Evan Wright tells how, during the second Gulf War, the U.S. Marines reacted to town lights about forty kilometers away. The autokinetic effect led them to believe that a large combat force was moving out to attack. An airstrike was called in for fifteen kilometers away, the estimated position of the lights. There was nothing there.

Motion-induced blindness in flight

The opposite of the autokinetic effect is motion-induced blindness, which causes objects to disappear from sight. A retired military aviator describes the phenomenon in an e-mail to RoadRUNNER magazine:

This is a great illustration of what we were taught about scanning outside the cockpit when I went through training.

We were told to scan the horizon for a short distance, stop momentarily, and repeat the process. I can remember being told why this was the most effective technique to locate other aircraft. It was emphasized repeatedly to not fix your gaze for more than a couple of seconds on any single object. The instructors, some of whom were veterans with years of experience, instructed us to continually “keep our eyes moving and our head on a swivel” because this was the best way to survive, not only in combat, but from peacetime hazards (like a midair collision) as well.

We basically had to take the advice on faith (until we could experience for ourselves) because the technology to demonstrate it didn’t exist at that time.

It’s easy to see this effect yourself by looking at one of the demonstrations online, which usually consist of a spinning grid with a single dot in the middle for focus and three yellow dots near the edges. There’s an example at www.michaelbach.de/ot/mot-mib.

If you move your eyes around as described by the pilot above, the yellow dots remain clearly in sight. But if you focus on the dot in the middle, you’ll discover that the dots disappear from view. Michael Bach has a configurable version of this test (found at the URL above), in which you can slow the movement right down and increase the size of the dots. They still disappear.

These two illusions alone mean that your fighter pilot flying through the stars has two problems. The autokinetic effect makes her think that the distant lights are incoming spacecraft coming to shoot her down. Meanwhile, motion-induced blindness makes the actual incoming space craft invisible to her.

Now that would be an interesting space battle!